Cognitive neuroscience Thomas D Albright*, Eric R Kandel† and Michael I Posner‡
The last decade of the 20th century has seen the development
disparate subfields of biology into one coherent disci-
of cognitive neuroscience as an effort to understand how the
pline. Almost all of the other unifications have been
brain represents mental events. We review the areas of
spearheaded by the synthetic power of molecular biology.
emotional and motor memory, vision, and higher mental
Cognitive neuroscience is distinctive in that the impor-
processes as examples of this new understanding. Progress
tant impetus has come from other sources; in particular, a
in all of these areas has been swift and impressive, but much
large part of the impetus has come from psychology and
needs to be done to reveal the mechanisms of cognition at the
local circuit and molecular levels. This work will require newmethods for controlling gene expression in higher animals and
That psychology should be a driving force for such a syn-
in studying the interactions between neurons at multiple levels.
thesis is perhaps not surprising. Psychology provides theagenda for the brain sciences; it poses the questions about
Addresses
mental activity that we ultimately want to address. In
*Howard Hughes Medical Institute and Salk Institute, 10010 North
addition, within the first half of the 20th century, psychol-
Torrey Pines Road, La Jolla, California 92037, USA;
ogy underwent a remarkable increase in explanatory
power, evolving from a philosophical tradition based on
† Howard Hughes Medical Institute and Center for Neurobiology andBehavior, College of Physicians and Surgeons of Columbia University,
introspection to an independent scientific discipline cen-
722 West 168th Street, New York, New York 10032, USA;
tered on psychophysics and behaviorism. As psychology
matured in the first half of the 20th century, it increasing-
‡ Sackler Institute, Department of Psychiatry, Box 140, Weill Medical
ly advocated an empirical behaviorist approach that ended
College of Cornell University, 1300 York Avenue, New York,New York 10021, USA; e-mail: mip2003@mail.med.cornell.edu
up reducing the focus of interest in psychology to observ-able aspects of behavior. Psychologists argued that
Current Opinion in Neurobiology 2000, 10:612–624
attempts to quantify non-observable mental events, such
as perception, imagery, thinking, retention, problem solv-
2000 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.
ing, or consciousness were at best speculative andunstable. This emphasis on observable indices of behav-
Abbreviations 5-HT
ior resulted in making psychology a rigorously
Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II
experimental discipline but one that was so narrowly
focused that it excluded from the study of behavior most
of the really fascinating features of mental life. The re-
awakening of interest in internal events led to the
emergence of modern cognitive psychology by making us
realize that our knowledge of the world is based on per-
ception, and that perception is an act of construction that
depends not only on the information inherent in the stim-
ulus but also on the mental structure of the perceiver.
Ulric Neisser laid out the task of cognitive psychology. In
his classic monograph of 1967 [1], he wrote:
“…the world of experience is produced by the man
who experiences it…There certainly is a real world
of trees and people and cars and even books, and it
has a great deal to do with our experience of these
objects. However, we have no direct immediate
tetracycline-responsive transcription factor
access to the world, nor to any of its properties…
Whatever we know about reality has been mediatednot only by the organs of sense but by complex sys-
Introduction
tems which interpret and reinterpret sensory
The last decade of the 20th century, the Decade of the
Brain, has also been the Decade of Cognitive
“. . . the term ‘cognition’ refers to all the processes
Neuroscience. It has been the decade in which the merg-
by which the sensory input is transformed, reduced,
er of cognitive psychology and neural science has begun
elaborated, stored, recovered and used . . .”
to realize its promise. The joining of neural science andcognitive psychology is the most recent in a series of sci-
Neisser, and the other earlier pioneers in cognitive psy-
entific unifications that have brought together the
chology, pointed out that to study mental operations,
Cognitive neuroscience Albright, Kandel and Posner 613
cognitive psychologists needed to focus on the flow of sen-
different sites within the brain. We briefly consider the
sory information, from its transduction by appropriate
systems problem before focusing on the molecular prob-
sensory receptors to its eventual use in memory and action.
lems of storage (for a current review, see [5]).
This implied that each perceptual or motor act has aninternal representation in the brain, evidenced by a pattern
The systems problem of memory
of activity in a specific set of interconnected cells. The pat-
Procedural (implicit) memory: there are multiple forms
tern of connections also stores information, in memory,
It is now generally accepted that declarative (explicit)
about the perception and the motor act.
memory requires for storage the medial temporal lobe andthe hippocampus, whereas procedural (implicit) memory
In the 1970s and 1980s, the early years of cognitive psychol-
does not. One of the advances of the past decade has been
ogy, most types of internal representations were not really
the further documentation that procedural memory has
accessible for a meaningful experimental analysis (for inter-
many subcomponents, and that essentially all cognitive
esting exceptions, see [2–4]). Fortunately, the emergence of
systems — be they perceptual or motor — involving a
cognitive psychology was soon followed by, and indeed
large variety of different neural systems may be capable of
helped stimulate, significant progress in systems neural sci-
storing some types of procedural memory. For example,
ence and in brain imaging. By the 1990s, these approaches
various types of motor learning involve the cerebellum;
had come together, making it possible, for the first time, to
learned fear the amygdala; operant conditioning and habit
study directly internal sensory and motor representations in
learning the basal ganglia; priming the neocortex; and per-
both nonhuman primates and human beings. A major goal of
ceptual learning, including habituation and sensitization,
this new direction was to map elementary cognitive func-
the primary sensory pathways. This distribution of proce-
tions onto specific neuronal systems.
dural memory across neural systems was already wellappreciated in the 1980s, and, in some cases, such as motor
We cannot, in this brief review, document the substantial
learning involving the cerebellum, the circuitry involved in
progress that has been made in mapping internal represen-
learning was already well studied (see reviews in [6–10]).
tations of cognitive functions. We therefore will restrict our
Here, we illustrate how our knowledge of implicit memory
review to three areas in which we have direct experience:
mechanisms has expanded in the 1990s by focusing briefly
memory storage, perception, and higher mental functions.
on just two examples, fear conditioning in the amygdala
Our purpose in considering memory storage is to illustrate
and motor learning in the cerebellum.
that some elementary aspects of cognitive processes, inparticular the switch from short- to long-term memory, can
now be studied at the molecular level. In discussing per-
Considerable evidence from both humans and experimen-
ception, we illustrate the influence that psychophysics,
tal animals now indicates that the amygdala is critical for
especially psychophysical studies in intact, awake behav-
the expression of emotion and intervenes between the
ing primates, has exerted on the study of the neuronal
hypothalamus and brainstem nuclei — the regions con-
organization and function of the visual system. Finally, we
cerned with the somatic expression of emotion — and the
illustrate the remarkable power that we have gained in the
cingulate, parahippocampal, and prefrontal cortices — the
study of complex cognitive processes in humans from the
neocortical areas concerned with conscious feeling. For
ability to image the living, behaving human brain.
example, electrical stimulation of the amygdala in humansproduces feelings of fear and apprehension, whereas dam-
Memory storage
age to the amygdala in experimental animals produces
During the past decade, the study of memory storage has
tameness. Consistent with this idea, studies using positron
been characterized by the attempt to map the molecular
emission tomography (PET) and functional magnetic res-
biology of synaptic plasticity onto ideas about memory
onance imaging (fMRI) clearly show that recognition of
systems that emerged from cognitive psychology in the
emotional expression in faces involves the amygdala [11].
1980s. The earlier, cognitive psychological studies ofpatients with brain lesions made it apparent that memory
As early as 1956, Lawrence Weiskrantz [12] had demon-
was not a unitary faculty of mind but had at least two
strated that lesions of the amygdala in experimental
major forms: a declarative (explicit) form concerned with
animals also impair learned fear. In subsequent work,
the knowledge of what something was about — a knowl-
Michael Davies [13], Michael Fanselow [14], Joseph
edge about facts and events — and a procedural (implicit)
LeDoux [15] and their colleagues have delineated the out-
form concerned with the knowledge of how to do some-
lines of a neural circuit for learned fear. They found that
thing — a knowledge about perceptual and motor
one subnucleus of the amygdala, the lateral nucleus, is
procedures. In studying these two independent memory
required for procedural memory of fear conditioning to a
processes, it has proven convenient to divide the study of
neutral tone [15]. Information about the CS, the tone, is
memory storage into two parts: the systems problem of
carried to the lateral nucleus via two pathways: the thalamo-
memory, which is concerned with where in the brain
amygdala projection from the auditory thalamus and the
memories are stored, and the molecular problem of mem-
cortico-amygdala projection from the auditory cortex.
ory, which is concerned with how memories are stored in
Long-term potentiation (LTP) has been observed in vivoA decade of Current neurobiology
in the projection from the thalamus to the amygdala after
the cerebellar cortex and the deep nuclei, with each playing
tetanization of the thalamic input [16]. Importantly, synap-
a different role. The cerebellar cortex has the special func-
tic change resembling LTP has been observed in this
tion of learning the timing of movement. In so doing, the
pathway after naturally occurring fear conditioning [17].
cerebellar cortex guides the learning in the deep nuclei,which may allow learning to be transferred from the cortex
Because of the anatomical complexity of the amygdala
to the deep nuclei. Since one of the issues in declarative
(especially when compared with more ordered structures
learning is the transfer of information from the hippocampus
such as the hippocampus and the cerebellum), precise
and the medial temporal lobe to other areas of neocortex,
molecular characterization of the plasticity at these defined
this insight may prove to be of general significance (see dis-
synapses has not yet been achieved. However, recent find-
cussion of Declarative memory storage below).
ings have begun to make some progress. Thus, blockade ofnoradrenaline receptors has been found to interfere with
Recent studies indicate that the cerebellum probably does
formation of emotional memory in humans, suggesting
even more than participate in motor learning; it also par-
that the cAMP pathway in the amygdala may be required
ticipates in associative word learning [29], learning of
[18]. Disruption of cAMP-dependent kinase (PKA) in fact
tactile mazes [30], and perception of time intervals
disrupts fear conditioning [19,20]. More recently, LTP has
been described in slices of the amygdala at both thesynapse from the thalamus and the cortex to the lateral
Declarative (explicit) memory storage: the medial temporal
amygdala, and, like LTP in the hippocampus, it has been
system appears to have only a temporary role
found to have an early phase and a protein-synthesis-
A key feature of declarative memory is that the medial tem-
dependent late phase [21]. The induction of the early
poral lobe is involved in memory for a limited period of
phase is postsynaptic, but the expression is presynaptic
time. The initial evidence came from studies of the patient
H.M., as well as the two other patients described byPenfield and Milner [32] who had good memory for remote
Memory for motor acts and the cerebellum
events that occurred years before their surgery. Recently,
Well into the 1970s and 1980s, many neural scientists still
studies in experimental animals have tested this idea rigor-
thought that the function of the cerebellum was restricted
ously and obtained clear evidence for temporally graded
to coordinating voluntary movements: gait, posture,
retrograde amnesia, which covered a period ranging from a
speech, and other skilled movements. This view began to
few days to about a month before surgery (reviewed in [5]).
change in the early 1980s, when a series of studies byRichard Thompson, Masao Ito, Mitchell Glickstein, Tom
These data have given rise to the idea that the medial tem-
Thach and their colleagues provided a variety of indepen-
poral lobe structures direct a gradual process of
dent evidence that the cerebellum is also critical for the
reorganization and stabilization by changing the organiza-
formation of learned motor responses ([6–9]; for early
tion of cortical representations, perhaps by binding together
theoretical discussions, see also [24,25]). For example,
the separate cortical regions that store memory for a whole
Thompson and co-workers [6,26,27] found that lesions of
event [33,34]. After sufficient time has passed, the hip-
the cerebellar cortex produced deficits in conditioned eye-
pocampal formation may not be needed to support storage
blink responses while sparing the unconditioned
or retrieval of declarative memory, and long-term memory
responses. The site for this plasticity appears to reside in
is fully dependent on the neocortex (reviewed in [35]).
the mossy-fiber–parallel-fiber Purkinje cell synapse [27]. Indeed, conditioned responses can be obtained by simply
The molecular mechanisms of memory storage
substituting for the CS direct electrical stimulation of
A role for CREB in procedural (implicit) and declarative
brainstem (pontine) nuclei and pairing that with electrical
stimulation of fibers from the inferior olive as the US [27].
Although declarative and nondeclarative memory use dif-ferent neural systems and different logic, the two
The work on eyeblink conditioning is part of a larger tra-
memory systems have in common distinct temporal phas-
dition of work on the role of the cerebellum in learning, a
es. There is a short-term memory lasting minutes to
tradition that includes the studies of Masao Ito [9] on the
hours, and a long-term memory lasting days or even
modification of the vestibular–ocular reflex and the studies
longer. In both memory systems, long-term memory dif-
of Thomas Thach [28] on the role of mossy and climbing
fers from short-term memory not only in time course but
fibers in monkeys learning to adjust their wrist movements
also in molecular mechanisms. Long-term memory, but
not short-term memory, requires the synthesis of newprotein (reviewed in [36,37]). Studies during the past
A comparison of learning in the vestibular–ocular reflex to
decade in Drosophila, Aplysia, and mice suggest that
learning of classical eyeblink conditioning by Raymond et al.
cAMP, PKA, and the cAMP-responsive transcription fac-
[10] suggests that these two quite different behaviors show
tor CREB are critically involved in the conversion of
a surprisingly conserved role for the cerebellum in motor
short-term to long-term memory for both declarative and
learning. In each case, the plasticity is distributed between
Cognitive neuroscience Albright, Kandel and Posner 615
Indeed, in both Drosophila and Aplysia, CREB learning-
protein synthesis and the kinase PKA [46]. Moreover,
related transcriptional activation appears to be the
there is increasing evidence for rapid receptor insertion
rate-limiting step in the conversion of short-term to long-
and remodeling during E-LTP and for actual structural
term synaptic plasticity and behavioral memory. This was
changes during L-LTP (reviewed in [47]).
first shown for synaptic plasticity by injecting into thenucleus of the sensory neuron of Aplysia an oligonucleotide
Development of gene targeting by homologous recombina-
with a cAMP-responsive element (CRE) [38]. This
tion in embryonic stem cells has made it possible to test
oligonucleotide selectively blocked long-term facilitation
some of these ideas genetically. The initial studies exam-
without affecting short-term facilitation. A similar result
ined mice with targeted knockout of CaMKII and the
was later obtained by injecting antibodies specific for
tyrosine kinase fyn, kinases that had previously been impli-
CREB1 [39]. Conversely, injecting a phosphorylated form
cated in LTP in pharmacological studies [44,48]. The
of the recombinant CREB1a protein, a key regulator of
genetic studies revealed that mice lacking CaMKIIα dis-
transcription, was sufficient to induce long-term facilita-
played a partial loss of E-LTP in the CA1 neurons of the
tion [40]. Injection of antibodies against Aplysia CREB2
hippocampus and severe impairment on spatial memory
(ApCREB2), a negative regulator that inhibits CREB1-
tasks [49–51]. Similarly, analysis of mice with targeted dele-
mediated transcription, led to long-term facilitation
tions of the tyrosine kinase fyn also showed deficits in
accompanied by a growth of new synaptic connections [40]
E-LTP as well as in spatial memory. Mice with deletions of
when paired with a single pulse of 5-HT (which normally
the non-receptor tyrosine kinases src and yes were normal
produces short-term facilitation). Thus, ApCREB2 acts as
[52]. By contrast, expression of a dominant-negative
a repressor of long-term facilitation by functionally com-
inhibitor of PKA in neurons of the forebrain using the
peting with the CREB1a activator. In parallel experiments
CaMKIIα promoter [53] resulted in mice that had a normal
in Drosophila, the behavioral switch from short- to long-
E−LTP but a dramatically attenuated L-LTP. These genet-
term memory was found to be regulated by changing the
ically modified mice learned a contextual task as well as
activity ratio between CRE binding activator dCREB2a
wild-type animals, had a perfectly good short-term memory
and repressor dCREB2b proteins, the fly homologues of
when tested 1 hour after training, but were impaired in
mammalian CREB and Aplysia CREB1 [36,41]. For exam-
selective long-term memory when tested 24 hours later.
ple, when the expression of the CREB activator is induced
Thus, as in Aplysia and Drosophila, the PKA signaling path-
before training, a single odor–shock pairing produces a
way in mice seems to be important for maintaining both
LTP and memory for prolonged periods of time.
These results in Drosophila suggest that this CREB-medi-
Previously, Roussoudan Bourtchouladze et al. [54] had
ated induction of transcription is necessary to produce the
examined the role of CREB in the mouse. They found that
long-lasting changes in synaptic strength required for the
a deletion that eliminated only the alpha and delta isoforms
long-term storage of memories [36,37]. The results in
led to impairment in LTP and in long-term memory storage
Aplysia suggest that the CREB switch functions at the level
[54]. The memory deficit was similar to that seen in mice
of the individual synapse to convert a short-lasting increase
expressing the PKA inhibitor; initial learning and short-
in synaptic strength produced by covalent modifications of
term memory were intact, whereas long-term memory was
existing proteins to one that is long-lasting and produced by
impaired. Moreover, work by Daniel Storm and colleagues
the synthesis of new proteins (see also [42,43]).
[55,56] has provided further strong evidence that CREB isindeed involved in mouse synaptic plasticity. They pro-
duced a transgenic mouse in which a lacZ reporter gene is
What about the switch to long-term declarative memory?
activated by a CREB-responsive promoter, and they found
In the past decade, methods for modifying individual
that this reporter is activated both by L-LTP in vitro [55]
genes in mice have become available and have helped
and by certain forms of hippocampus-dependent learning
define the mouse as the mammalian model system parin vivo [56]. This demonstrates that CREB or CREB-like
excellence for the genetic study of declarative memory stor-
transcription factors are in fact activated under circum-
age. Mice exhibit a memory for space and objects that
stances that lead to plasticity and suggests a causal role
corresponds to human declarative memory, requiring the
similar to that seen in Aplysia and Drosophila.
hippocampus and the medial temporal lobe. Moreover,the hippocampus has a form of synaptic plasticity, called
Regionally and temporally restricted gene expression
LTP, thought to be a candidate mechanism for this sort of
The traditional genetically modified mouse lines men-
memory storage. Pharmacological experiments in the
tioned above have identified some genes that are necessary
1980s and early 1990s first indicated that LTP itself has
to develop a normal learning and memory phenotype in the
stages, much like long-term facilitation in Aplysia. There
adult, and suggest genes that may vary in their allelic form
is an early-stage LTP (E-LTP) that requires covalent
in a normal population to give rise to the normal variation in
modification mediated by Ca2+/calmodulin-dependent
cognitive ability. These lines of mice, however, suffer from
protein kinase II α (CaMKIIα) and the tyrosine kinase
the limitation that many molecules likely to be important
fyn [44,45], and a late-phase LTP (L-LTP) that requires
for adult learning are also likely to be important for normal
A decade of Current neurobiology
development. In addition, these gene manipulations often
Overexpression of calcineurin leads to an impairment in an
affect a variety of different brain regions. One example of
intermediate form of LTP (I−LTP) in hippocampus and to
how to circumvent this problem is provided by work on
a defect in spatial memory in the Morris water maze
regional and regulated gene expression in the brain [57–59].
[63,64,66]. Since the transgene can readily be switched onand off by giving or removing doxycycline, it has the great
Mice with a targeted deletion in the NR1 subunit of the
advantage that it can be used to study not only memory
NMDA receptor die shortly after birth [60]. Previously, Joe
storage but also memory retrieval. Mutant mice that
Tsien and colleagues [57,58] succeeded in obtaining condi-
express the calcineurin transgene transiently, after learning
tional deletion restricted to forebrain neurons by using the
has been acquired and after spatial memory has already
CRE-loxP systems. By combining the forebrain-specific
been stored, have an apparent defect in the retrieval of the
CaMKIIα promoter with the bacterial CRE-loxP recombi-
spatial information. This retrieval defect is not attributable
nase system, they were able to knock out the NR1 gene
to a disruption in memory storage because it could be
specifically in the CA1 region of the postnatal hippocampus,
reversed when the transgene expression was turned off by
without affecting other structures. This restricted knockout
stopping doxycycline administration. Thus, with the use of
avoided the perinatal lethality of a complete NR1 knockout.
regulated genetic modification one can not only control for
As predicted, the mice showed a deficit in CA1 LTP and a
potential developmental abnormalities associated with a
severe deficit in spatial learning, strongly supporting a role
genetic change but also begin to explore the various
for NMDA-dependent LTP in hippocampus-dependent
phases of memory acquisition, storage and retrieval [64].
learning. More recently, Tsien and colleagues [61] have car-ried out the reverse experiment. They expressed a mutant
Future directions
form of NR1 that allows greater Ca2+ influx, and found that
The use of mouse genetics to investigate complex behav-
ioral traits such as learning and memory is at an early stage,but it promises to extend cognitive neuroscience into a
A further advance has been the ability to regulate gene
new, molecular genetic direction. Moreover, the work in
expression not just regionally but also temporally, using
both invertebrates and mice suggests that many of the
the tetracycline system (see e.g. [62]). In a group of exper-
basic molecular mechanisms for memory may be con-
iments, a cell-type-specific promoter, such as the
served across species, allowing insights from invertebrates
CaMKIIα promoter, is used to drive expression of the
to be applied to the mammalian brain. In the fly, for exam-
tetracycline-responsive transcription factor tTA in fore-
ple, the characterization of a new learning and memory
brain neurons of one line of mice. In a second line, the
mutants will hopefully provide a fuller understanding of
transgene to be regulated is linked to a promoter consist-
the critical genes involved in memory storage. However,
ing of multiple repeats of the tet operator (tetO) linked to
since the developmental integrity of many brain regions is
a minimal eukaryotic promoter element. When these two
necessary for the proper performance of even the simplest
lines are mated so that both transgenes are introduced into
memory task, and since memory storage requires some of
a single mouse, the tetO-linked gene is activated specifi-
the most basic cellular signaling mechanisms, there will be
cally in those cells that express tTA. The expression of the
many developmentally important genes that affect learn-
tetO-linked transgene can then be suppressed by oral
ing and memory. The difficult task facing the field,
administration of the tetracycline analogue doxycycline
therefore, will be to distinguish those mutations that affect
[59]. In another class of experiments, a mutant form of the
the core cellular signaling mechanisms that are used to
tetracycline repressor is used that induces transcription
encode memories from those mechanisms that modulate
only in the presence of doxycycline [62]. This reverse tTA,
these mechanisms or affect the development of the basic
or rtTA, has been used to obtain inducible and reversible
circuits important for performing the learning task or for
expression of a Ca2+-activated protein phosphatase cal-
storing the learned information. The use of progressively
cineurin in forebrain neurons [63,64]. Calcineurin opposes
better anatomically restricted and temporally regulated
the action of PKA and other protein kinases. Calcineurin
genetic modification in the mouse will be critical for dis-
has a high affinity for Ca2+, even higher than that of
tinguishing those genes that directly affect memory
CaMKIIα. At low-frequency stimulation, the amount of
encoding from those that affect memory indirectly through
Ca2+ coming into the cell through the NMDA receptor is
developmental, motivational, or perceptual mechanisms.
small and activates calcineurin but not CaMKIIα. Calcineurin, in turn, can dephosphorylate protein phos-
phatase Inhibitor-1, which activates protein phosphatase 1
In his pioneering text, which first appeared 50 years ago,
(PP1) and leads to long-term depression (LTD) of synaptic
Donald Hebb [67] observed that “we know virtually noth-
transmission, which is the mirror opposite of LTP [65]. In
ing about what goes on between the arrival of an excitation
contrast, higher frequencies of stimulation lead to greater
at a sensory projection area and its later departure from the
Ca2+ influx. This, in turn, activates kinases, including
motor area of the cortex…” “Something like thinking inter-
PKA, that phosphorylate and block Inhibitor-1, thereby
venes,” and although it would be hard to disagree with that
shutting off the phosphatase cascade. In fact, PKA and cal-
proposition, the goal of cognitive neuroscience has been to
cineurin target the same residue on Inhibitor-1.
flesh out that ‘something’ in a form that is more satisfying
Cognitive neuroscience Albright, Kandel and Posner 617
to both psychologists and neurobiologists alike. In part
In a set of experiments conducted by Thomas Albright,
because its operations span the chasm that Hebb lament-
Gene Stoner, and colleagues [71–73], contextual cues
ed, the visual system has served as a proving ground for
unrelated to visual motion (e.g. luminance or stereoscopic
this goal. By tracing the flow of visual information from
cues that elicit a percept of surface depth ordering) were
retina to motor control circuits we can, in principle, deter-
found to markedly alter perceived motion, even though
mine how its representation by the brain contributes to the
retinal stimulus motion remained unchanged. In addition,
various cognitive processes that constitute thinking, such
the responses of many motion-sensitive neurons in cortical
as perception, recognition, imagery, decision making, and
visual area MT were found to co-vary with perceived
motion rather than with retinal motion, demonstrating thatthe formation of perceptual or ‘scene-based’ neuronal rep-
The 1990s will long be remembered as a turning point in
resentations is accomplished at early stages in the visual
this effort. As often recorded in these pages, recent
advances in cognitive neuroscience are many. This state ofaffairs owes much to the fact that neurobiologists have
Related evidence for scene-based representations has
with increasing frequency turned to experimental psychol-
come from experiments in which perceived features are
ogy (and vice versa) for guidance, inspiration, and tools.
not physically present in the retinal stimulus, but their
Products of this new-found interdisciplinary success
presence in the visual scene is implied by contextual cues.
include discoveries regarding the correspondence between
In one such study, Rüdiger von der Heydt and colleagues
neuronal and perceptual events, the role of context in per-
[76] studied the neuronal basis of a perceptual phenome-
ceptual processing, the neuronal substrates of attention
non known as ‘illusory contours’, in which contextual cues
and decision making, the plasticity of adult sensory repre-
imply the presence of an occluding surface and the edges
sentations, and the role of such in perceptual learning and
of the surface are seen, even though they are not actually
sensory–motor spatial coordinate transformations.
present in the stimulus. These investigators discoveredthat many neurons fire in a way that matches the percept,
Linking neuronal and perceptual events
as though a real contour had been placed in the receptive
We entered the past decade secure in the knowledge that
field. In another study, John Assad and John Maunsell [77]
the stimulus selectivities of visual neurons resemble the
exploited the fact that observers generally infer the con-
basic elements of perceptual experience, such as simple
tinuous motion of an object when it moves behind an
forms, motions, and colors. Lacking, however, was evidence
occluder. By using remote contextual cues to place the
for a specific causal relationship between neuronal and per-
path of occluded motion within the receptive fields of
ceptual events. The problem was remedied by William
motion-sensitive MT neurons, these investigators found
Newsome and colleagues, who united classic methods from
that many neurons respond in a way that matches the per-
experimental psychology with modern neurobiological
ceptual inference of motion, in the absence of any real
techniques (for a review, see [68]). These investigators dis-
motion in the receptive field. The significance of these
covered a close relationship between perceptual motion
findings lies, of course, in the fact that the neuronal rep-
sensitivity and the sensitivity of neurons in cortical visual
resentations reflect — as does perception — the enduring
area MT [69], which strongly suggested that neuronal activ-
structural and relational qualities of the observer’s
ity within area MT is a constituent of the perceptual
experience of motion. Icing came in the form of anotherstudy in which Newsome and colleagues modified the per-
Visual attention
ceptual experience of motion by artificially activating small
The primate visual system has a limited information pro-
collections of MT neurons [70]. The results from these
cessing capacity. An exciting area of research in the 1990s
experiments have offered sound reassurance that the stim-
has been that addressing the means and conditions under
ulus selectivities of visual neurons account for perceptual
which this limited capacity — visual attention — is
experience, and they have paved the way for an under-
dynamically allocated. Work in this area has revealed two
standing of the underlying mechanisms.
basic types of attentional phenomena, which may havedistinct neuronal substrates. One effect, known as ‘atten-
Sensation versus perception
tional facilitation’, is the improved processing of a
The Newsome experiments solved one puzzle, but as
stimulus when it appears at an attended location. Early
many neurobiologists of the 1990s gained sophistication in
investigations of the effects of focal brain lesions in
perceptual psychology, they were forced to confront another:
humans implicated the parietal lobe in attentional facili-
as understood up to that point, the stimulus selectivities of
tation. In subsequent physiological studies of parietal
visual neurons encoded properties of the retinal stimulus.
cortex in non-human primates, Michael Goldberg and
But things perceived reflect the ‘meaning’ of the stimulus,
colleagues [79] found that for many neurons an attended
as defined by the content of the visual scene that leads to
visual stimulus elicited a much larger sensory response
its appearance. Several studies carried out during the past
than did an identical unattended stimulus. Similar facili-
decade have sought to distinguish neuronal representa-
tatory effects have since been reported for other cortical
tions of sensory and perceptual events. A decade of Current neurobiology
The other basic attentional effect that has been studied
search for neuronal responses that ‘predict’ an impending
extensively is known as ‘attentional selection’. This effect
motor response to a visual cue (see e.g. [89]). In a recent
refers to the phenomenon in which a target stimulus (i.e.
study, Michael Shadlen and colleagues [90] exploited the
the thing you’re looking for) is selected from among other
fact that difficult decisions generally require time to accu-
stimuli that are competing for attention. In the mid-1980s,
mulate relevant information, hence predictive neurons
Robert Desimone and colleagues [82] found that receptive
should exhibit responses that increase in magnitude in par-
field profiles of individual neurons in cortical areas V4 and
allel with the observer’s decision confidence. These
IT contract around the attended stimulus, excluding unat-
investigators found that neurons in prefrontal cortex do
tended stimuli. These findings of selection at the neuronal
exactly that. Collectively, these novel studies of the deci-
level imply that information about an attended stimulus is
sion process have identified relevant neuronal substrates
carried to higher processing stages, at the expense of infor-
and have led to promising theories regarding ways in which
mation about unattended stimuli. Selective effects have
visual information is flexibly mapped to action.
now been reported for many visual areas, including areasV1, V2, V4, MT, MST, and IT (see e.g. [83–86]), indicat-
Perceptual learning
ing that selective mechanisms operate simultaneously on
It is a central tenet of neurobiology that the sensory neo-
cortex reaches its mature state of organization following abrief period of postnatal plasticity known as the critical
Physiological studies are beginning to target the underly-
period. One of the most important discoveries of the past
ing mechanisms of selective attention. Much of the work
decade, however, is the large extent to which this plastici-
in this area has been inspired by the ‘biased competition’
ty continues throughout life. Adult plasticity enables forms
model advocated by Desimone and Duncan [87].
of cognitive flexibility such as perceptual learning, which
According to this view, pieces of incoming sensory infor-
is the improvement with practice in the ability to discrim-
mation compete for neuronal representation (and,
inate sensory attributes. Early hints of this plastic potential
ultimately, control of the observer’s actions), and the com-
came from studies demonstrating that the adult cortex
petition is biased such that behaviorally relevant inputs are
undergoes a local functional reorganization to compensate
facilitated. Support for this model comes from a recent
for damage to the sensory periphery [91–93]. This com-
study by Desimone and colleagues [86], who found that
pensation may be mediated by intrinsic cortical
the neuronal response to two unattended stimuli placed
connections, which appear to undergo rapid changes in
together in the receptive field was approximately the aver-
synaptic efficacy, as well as a slower process of sprouting
age of the responses elicited by the two stimuli presented
and synaptogenesis [94,95]. The belief that this form of
independently. By contrast, when either stimulus was
plasticity also underlies adult perceptual learning is sup-
attended — thus giving it a competitive advantage — the
ported by evidence that training on perceptual tasks leads
neuronal response approximated that elicited by the
to reorganization of cortical sensory maps [96] and
attended stimulus alone. Although these findings beg
improvements in the sensitivities of cortical neurons [97].
important and difficult questions regarding the source ofthe biasing signal, as well as the local circuit and synaptic
Transforming signals from visual space to motor space
interactions that lead to changes in the receptive field pro-
Another success story from cognitive neuroscience in the
file, they provide a striking example of the gains afforded
1990s centers on the problem of converting visual inputs to
by the convergence of psychology and neurobiology.
signals that can guide actions. One long-standing viewholds that retinal signals, which represent visual space in a
Perceptual decisions
coordinate frame that shifts with every movement of the
While intelligent behavior depends upon knowledge of
eyes, are re-mapped into a more generic and stable coordi-
one’s external environment (i.e. perception), it also
nate frame based, for example, on the positions of objects
requires deciding which actions are appropriate given that
relative to the observer’s head or body. Consistent with this
knowledge. This ‘decision process’ has been the focus of
view, Richard Andersen and colleagues [98] discovered that
several revealing experiments over the past decade. One
the magnitude of response to a visual stimulus varies with
goal has been to identify neuronal activity that is correlat-
the angle of gaze. Because they take eye position into
ed with the decision to execute a particular action in
account, these ‘gain field’ responses yield a head-centered
response to a particular sensory stimulus, rather than sim-
map of visual space that is distributed across a population of
ply correlated with either stimulus or action alone. In a
parietal neurons [99]. More recent studies have obtained
series of studies, Earl Miller and colleagues [88] found that
intriguing evidence for explicit cellular representations of
the responses of neurons in prefrontal cortex — an area
space in pre-motor cortex. Carl Olson and Sonya Gettner
long believed to play a role in the organization of complex
[100], for example, found that some premotor neurons rep-
behavior — change as new sensory–motor relationships are
resent spatial location relative to the parts of a visible object
learned, such that individual neurons come to represent
(i.e. ‘object-based’ coordinates), independent of the posi-
new behaviorally relevant conjunctions of stimulus and
tion of the object’s image on the retina. Perhaps even more
action. Other groups have adopted a different approach to
remarkable are the findings of Michael Graziano, Charles
the decision process, the principal feature of which is a
Gross and colleagues [101], who reported the existence of
Cognitive neuroscience Albright, Kandel and Posner 619
premotor neurons that represent the position of a visual
their organization into circuits, their pathologies and
stimulus relative to the position of the observer’s forearm.
Body-part-centered representations of this sort appear wellsuited for orchestrating specific limb movements to stimuli
Imaging has strengthened the correspondences between the brain anatomy of humans and that of experimental animals The binding problem
A major advance in making cross-species comparisons has
In reviewing major themes of cognitive neuroscience
been the development of flat maps that provide a two-
research in the 1990s, we would be remiss to exclude a
dimensional surface for mapping the complex folds of the
topic that has captured enormous attention in disciplines
human brain [106]. There has been progress in efforts to
ranging from visual physiology to philosophy, while
relate retinotopic human visual areas to the maps
nonetheless remaining one of the most unsettled. The
obtained from cellular recording in primates [107].
topic in question is the use of temporal binding codes to
Evidence that attention enhances activity in V1 (for a
represent complex conjunctions of information carried by
review, see [108]), has made it possible to explore atten-
individual neurons. The potential utility of such codes has
tional influences in a brain area where the detailed
long been recognized. Empirical support came in the form
cellular structure is better understood. Cellular studies in
of physiological data from Charles Gray, Wolf Singer and
monkeys indicate the importance of attention in integrat-
colleagues [102], which suggested that visual features (e.g.
ing visual effects that occur outside the classic receptive
edges) perceived as parts of the same object are represent-
field of V1 neurons, for example, in perceiving contours
ed by neurons that fire synchronously. Others, however,
[109]. The interaction of attention with V1 circuitry may
have failed to find such support (or have raised objections
also be important in understanding the early visual sys-
on theoretical grounds), and the unprecedented polariza-
tem plasticity described in the previous section.
tion of opinions on the subject remains palpable as thedecade draws to a close (e.g. see reviews in the October
Integration of human and animal studies is not limited to
1999 issue of Neuron). In light of the importance of the
the early visual system. Studies of parietal neurons, locat-
binding problem for both a functional and mechanistic
ed within brain areas shown to be active in imaging studies
understanding of cognition, and the attention and
of spatial attention [110,111], have provided evidence of a
resources that have been directed at the problem in recent
map of locations organized by their current importance
years, this persistent lack of consensus is both surprising
[112] that could serve as the basis for human working
and greatly disappointing. One can only hope that resolu-
memory for location [113]. Using new fMRI methods, it
tion will come from the application of new concepts and
has been possible to separate operations performed by the
superior and inferior areas of the parietal lobe. Theseresults [111] suggest that the temporal–parietal junction is
Imaging higher cognitive functions
critical for shifts of attention toward unexpected visual
Imaging has been critical for the localization of mental
stimuli, a finding that supports the importance of this area
processes
in the neglect of space opposite the lesioned hemisphere
As one reads journals devoted to cognitive neuroscience or
found in patients suffering from strokes that affect the
human brain mapping, it is hard to imagine that there
could ever have been doubts that there was a specificanatomy related to higher mental processes. Only in the
Timing and the functional connectivity of neural circuits
past decade have neuroimaging studies using PET and
Of equal importance to precision in space is the ability to
fMRI demonstrated a pervasive form of localization in a
say exactly when and for how long an anatomical area is
wide variety of cognitive and emotional tasks [103,104].
active and when information is being exchanged between
The localization of mental operations, as described in the
areas. Because mental operations occur in the range of tens
first section of this review, has made psychology a full
to hundreds of milliseconds, it has been useful to relate
partner in efforts to understand human brain mechanisms.
areas of activity found in imaging studies to the distribu-tion of electrical activity recorded from the scalp [114] or
The distributed nature of the activations in any real cog-
by depth electrodes [115]. For example, using combined
nitive task helps explain why Karl Lashley [105] and
ERP and fMRI methods, it has been shown that although
others could have thought that the brain operated as a
attention influences activity in primary visual cortex, this
whole. However, in tasks involving language, mental
takes place only after the information has been processed
imagery, spatial navigation and working memory, where
we have been able to dissect them into plausible compu-tations, it is these components not the task themselves
A different approach to the circuitry of high-level cognition
that are localized. Of course localization is only a start
is to examine functional connectivity by studying the cor-
toward the achievement of cognitive neuroscience. As
relation between brain areas on the basis of hemodynamic
described below, imaging has provided an important
[117] or electro-magnetic [118] measurements. These
impetus for exploring the evolution of mental operations,
efforts provide an approach to the transfer of information
A decade of Current neurobiology
between brain areas by specifying within a task exactly the
Neuroimaging studies have provided us with some mech-
time when their activity is correlated.
anisms by which experience at different time scales mightchange anatomy or circuitry on a temporary or permanent
Recently, an adaptation of magnetic imaging called diffu-
basis. One way in which brain circuitry can be altered is
sion tensor imaging has been used to produce images of
called priming, which refers to changes in the efficiency of
the white matter connecting brain areas [119,120]. The
processing a target when part or all of the pathway involved
time course of myelination of neuronal pathways can be
has been previously activated. Combined neuroimaging
used to test theories of when in development particular
and cellular studies show that priming works by reducing
behaviors emerge. Perhaps this method, when combined
or tuning the number of neurons required to process the
with measuring correlations in electrical activity, will allow
target [131]. Studies in human subjects have shown that
us to predict when a given brain circuit is sufficiently
priming can take place within a second and that it may
developed to support the learning of complex skills. In a
take place even when the person is unaware of the identi-
recent study [121], for example, differences in white mat-
ty of the prime [132]. Priming may help account for
ter tracts in the temporal parietal area of the left
moment-to-moment differences in the thoughts generated
hemisphere were related to reading skill within both a nor-
in response to a given environmental challenge or strate-
mal and a reading-impaired population. Pathology: the search for subtle functional changes in
A few minutes of practice has been shown to be sufficient to
change the circuit that processes information from one that
Brain damage due to strokes and tumors can be observed
involves a high level of complex computation to one that has
readily in structural images, but other abnormalities may
an already compiled answer waiting for output [123,133].
involve more subtle functional changes. The use of struc-
Changes of pathways can help explain the shifts that take
tural images to map brain lesions has allowed investigators
place when extensive practice renders a skill automatic.
to combine the data from sets of patients with large cere-bral lesions showing by the overlap in lesion location the
Somewhat slower are the task-related increases in cerebral
brain areas that appear to cause the deficit. For example, a
tissue found in sensory [134] and motor [135] systems with
set of patients with large lesions of the left hemisphere all
extended practice. These changes in visual areas were dis-
showed expressive aphasia provided that they had a loss of
cussed in the previous section. The effects of extensive
neurons in the anterior insula [122]. This finding support-
practice have also been documented in studies of adults
ed results obtained with PET showing that the insula is an
who learned languages either as children or later in life
important pathway for highly automated verbal output,
[136] and musicians who had extensive practice on musical
instruments [137]. Although there are strong hints that therelative plasticity of these skills may differ between chil-
Tasks involving monitoring of emotion and cognition acti-
dren and adults, full documentation of this form of critical
vate separate areas of the frontal midline [124–126]. In
some studies, the cognitive and emotional areas appear tobe mutually inhibitory, with cognitive tasks reducing blood
The next decade?
flow in areas related to emotion, as well as the reverse
Donald Hebb and his fellow pioneers in cognitive neural
[126]. These areas appear to be important in some forms of
science would surely be pleased at the promiscuous bed-
psychopathology. For example, PET scans of patients with
fellows that psychologists and neurobiologists have now
schizophrenia who have never been on medication indicat-
become, and with their offspring, which is a fuller demon-
ed an abnormality in the left globus pallidus [127], which
stration that specific cognitive information is represented
is the outflow of a major dopamine circuit that modulates
in the activities of specific neuronal populations. These
cellular activity in midline frontal areas such as the anteri-
recent successes notwithstanding, it seems likely that
or cingulate gyrus [128]. This abnormal function might
Hebb would also recognize the weakness in our current
help to explain both the curious neglect of the right side of
view: lack of information about how these neuronal repre-
space shown in early schizophrenia and the changes in cir-
sentations are achieved mechanistically. How, for example,
cuitry within the anterior cingulate observed in the brains
are contextual cues assessed by cortical neurons to form
of schizophrenic patients postmortem [129].
visual representations that coincide with perceptual expe-rience? And how are such representations altered as a
Plasticity and shaping of neural circuits by experience
function of experience? Answers to these and other mech-
A major achievement of infancy research in the past
anistic questions require, at the very least, detailed
decade has been to show that human infants enter the
information about the patterns of anatomical connections
world with some mechanisms related to the processing of
in the cerebral cortex, and the functional properties con-
language, objects, faces and numbers, as well as with the
ferred by specific circuit components.
ability to imitate motor routines (for a review, see [130]). How does experience shape these initial mechanisms into
Determining the local circuit organization of the cerebral
cortex and how that organization relates to the processing
Cognitive neuroscience Albright, Kandel and Posner 621
of region-specific information is, however, dauntingly
powerful molecular and genetic tools find their calling in
complex, and its elucidation — important though it may
the service of cognitive neuroscience, and that the field will
be — is among the most formidable challenges facing cog-
continue to advance through a global circuit-based
nitive neuroscience in the next decade. Hope lies in some
approach to cognitive representation by the brain. These
remarkable new experimental approaches, which promise
are indeed heady times for the young field of cognitive
both fine-scale assessment of functional circuitry in the
neuroscience. Although, as noted by Hebb 50 years ago,
cortex and pictures of the global patterns of neuronal activ-
there still is “a long way to go before we can speak of under-
ity associated with specific cognitive states.
standing the principles of behavior to the degree that weunderstand the principles of chemical reaction”, the time
One of the most exciting prospects for fine-scale analysis
for that understanding is now — at least — in full view.
of functional circuitry can be found in methods for region-ally restricted and temporally regulated control of gene
Acknowledgements
expression. These methods took the stage in the 1990s in
We thank Marc Tessier-Lavigne for his helpful editing of this paper. TD Albright and ER Kandel are Investigators of the Howard Hughes
the form of mouse germ line transgenic manipulations, and
Medical Institute. The work of MI Posner was supported by a grant from
they have become extremely powerful tools for analysis of
the James S McDonnell Foundation to the Sackler Institute.
the cellular and molecular bases of learning and memory. Can we reasonably expect to use these new molecular tools
References
to study neural systems in animals such as non-human pri-
Neisser U: Cognitive Psychology. New York: Appleton-Century
mates, animals that possess a rich and human-like
repertoire of cognitive skills, but for which germ line gene
Mountcastle VB: Central nervous mechanisms in
transfer is all but impossible? Much cause for optimism
mechanoreceptive sensibility. In Handbook of Physiology, Section 1: The Nervous System, Vol. III: Sensory Processes, Part 2. Edited by
stems from newly developed methods that employ viruses
Darian-Smith I. Bethesda, Maryland: American Physiological Society;
as vectors for gene transfer, which promise to eliminate
dependence upon germ line transgenics and thus allow
Wurtz RH, Goldberg ME, Robinson DL: Brain mechanisms of visual
technology for temporal and cell-type specific gene regula-
attention. Sci Am 1982, 246:124-135.
tion to be imported to primates. Imagine, for example, the
O’Keefe J, Dostrovsky J: The hippocampus as a spatial map:
incredible possibilities for analysis of functional circuit
preliminary evidence from unit activity in the freely moving rat. Brain Res 1971, 34:171-175.
components afforded by the ability to switch on and off a
Milner B, Squire LR, Kandel ER: Cognitive neuroscience and the
specific class of cells in a specific cortical area while a mon-
study of memory. Neuron 1998, 3:3445-3468.
Thompson RF: Neural mechanisms of classical conditioning in mammals. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1990, 329:161-170.
At the other end of the continuum of mechanistic issues,
Thach WT, Goodkin HP, Keating JG: Cerebellum and the adaptive
we face the problem of measuring and interpreting the
coordination of movement. Annu Rev Neurosci 1992, 15:403-442.
interactions between individual neurons and between
Glickstein M, Yeo C: The cerebellum and motor learning. J Cogn
groups of neurons. Work of the past decade attests that the
Neurosci 1990, 2:69-80.
single neuron approach to perception and cognition has
Ito M: Long-term depression. Annu Rev Neurosci 1989, 12:85-102.
been profoundly successful. As we move beyond the rep-
10. Raymond JL, Lisberger STG, Mauk M: The cerebellum: a neuronal
resentational phenomenology of single neurons to confront
learning machine. Science 1996, 272:1126-1131.
mechanistic questions, however, we find that it is impossi-
11. Morris JS, Frith CD, Perrett DI, Rowland D, Young AN, Calder AJ,
ble to proceed without a more global approach to neural
Dolan RJ: A differential neural response in the human amygdala to fearful and happy facial expressions. Nature 1996, 383:812-815.
coding. If, for example, we expect to understand themechanisms that underlie the interactive contributions of
12. Weiskrantz L: Behavioral changes associated with ablation of the amygdala complex in monkeys. J Comp Physiol Psychol 1956,
a stimulus in the visual field and of working memory to
49:381-391.
selective visual attention, then it is essential that we be
13. Davis M: The role of the amygdala in fear and anxiety. Annu Rev
able to monitor simultaneously neuronal events in all the
Neurosci 1992, 15:353-375.
relevant interconnected brain regions. This is not an easy
14. Fanselow M: Neural organization of the defensive behavior system
task, nor is the associated problem of interpreting the mul-
responsible for fear. Psychonomic Bull Rev 1994, 1:429-438.
titude of potential neuronal interactions. New techniques
15. LeDoux JE: Fear in the brain, where have we been and where are we going? Biol Psychiatry 1998, 44:1229-1238.
based on multielectrode recording, optical imaging of neu-ronal activity, and fMRI — each used in conjunction with
16. Clugnet M, LeDoux JD: Synaptic plasticity in fear conditioning circuits: induction of LTP in the lateral nucleus of the amygdala by
the behavioral methods that have proved so valuable over
stimulation of the medial geniculate body. J Neurosci 1990,
the past decade — are beginning to make important
10:2818-2824.
Rogan MT, Staubli UV, LeDoux JE: Fear conditioning induces associative long-term potentiation in the amygdala. Nature 1997, 390:604-607.
In the decade of the 1990s, cognitive neuroscience thrived
18. Cahill L, Prins B, Weber M, McGaugh JL: Beta-adrenergic activation
by bringing together psychology and neurobiology. We now
and memory for emotional events. Nature 1994, 371:702-704.
have every reason to expect that the next decade will yield
19. Bourtchouladze R, Abel T, Berman N, Gordon R, Lapidus K,
a similarly mature molecular biology of cognition, in which
Kandel ER: Different training procedures recruit either one or two A decade of Current neurobiology critical periods for contextual memory consolidation, each of
41. Yin JC, Wallach JS, Del Vecchio M, Wilder EL, Zhou H, Quinn WG,
which requires protein synthesis and PKA. Learn Mem 1998,
Tully T: Induction of a dominant negative CREB transgene 5:365-374. specifically blocks long-term memory in Drosophila. Cell 1994, 79:49-58.
20. Schafe GE, Nadel NV, Sullivan GM, Harris A, LeDoux JE: Memory consolidation for contextual and auditory fear conditioning is
42. Martin KC, Casadio A, Zhu H, Yaping E, Rose J, Bailey CH, Chen M,
dependent on protein synthesis, PKA, and MAP kinase. Learn
Kandel ER: Synapse-specific transcription-dependent long-term Mem 1999, 6:97-110. facilitation of the sensory to motor neuron connection in Aplysia: a function for local protein synthesis in memory storage. Cell
21. Huang Y-Y, Martin KC, Kandel ER: Both PKA and MAP kinase are
1998, 91:927-938. required for the macromolecular synthesis-dependent late phase of LTP in the amygdala. J Neurosci 2000, in press.
43. Casadio A, Martin KC, Giustetto M, Zhu H, Chen M, Bartsch D,
Bailey CH, Kandel ER: A transient neuron-wide form of CREB-
22. Huang Y-Y, Kandel ER: Postsynaptic induction and PKA-dependent mediated long-term facilitation can be stabilized at specific expression of LTP in the lateral amygdala. Neuron 1998, synapses by local protein synthesis. Cell 1999, 99:221-237. 21:169-178.
44. Nicoll RA, Kauer JHA, Malenka RC: The current excitement in long-
23. Weisskopf MG, Bauer EB, LeDoux JE: L-type voltage-gated calcium term potentiation. Neuron 1988, 1:97-103. channels mediate NMDA independent associative long-term potentiation at thalamic input synapses to the amygdala.
45. Bliss TV, Collingridge GL: A synaptic model of memory: long-term J Neurosci 1999, 19:10512-10519. potentiation in the hippocampus. Nature 1993, 361:31-39.
24. Marr D: A theory of the cerebellar cortex. J Physiol 1969, 202:437-470.
46. Frey U, Huang Y-Y, Kandel ER: Effects of cAMP simulate a late stage of LTP in hippocampal CA1 neurons. Science 1993,
25. Albus JS: A theory of cerebellar function. Mol Biosci 1971, 10:25-61. 260:1661-1664.
26. McCormick DA, Lavond DG, Clark GA, Kettner RE, Rising CE,
Lüscher C, Nicoll R, Malenka RC, Muller D: Synaptic plasticity and
Thompson RF: The engram found? Role of the cerebellum in dynamic modulation of the postsynaptic membrane. Nat Neurosci classical conditioning of nictitating membrane and eyelid
2000, 3:545-550. responses. Bull Psychol Soc 1981, 18:103-105.
48. O’Dell TJ, Kandel ER, Grant SN: Long-term potentiation in the
Thompson RF, Thompson JK, Kim JJ, Krupa DJ, Shinkman PG: The hippocampus is blocked by tyrosine kinase inhibitors. Nature nature of reinforcement in cerebellar learning. Neurobiol Learn
1991, 353:558-560. Mem 1998, 70:150-176.
49. Silva AJ, Paylor R, Wehner JM, Tonegawa S: Impaired spatial
28. Thach WT: Correlation of neural discharge with pattern and force learning in alpha-calcium-calmodulin kinase II mutant mice. of muscular activity, joint position, and direction of intended next movement in motor cortex and cerebellum. J Neurophysiol 1978, Science 1992, 257:206-211. 41:654-676.
50. Silva AJ, Stevens CF, Tonegawa S, Wang Y: Deficient hippocampal
29. Fiez JA, Petersen SE, Cheney MK, Raichle ME: Impaired non-motor long-term potentiation in alpha-calcium-calmodulin kinase II learning and error detection associated with cerebellar damage. mutant mice. Science 1992, 257:201-206. Brain 1992, 115:155-178.
51. Malinow R: Silencing the controversy in LTP? Neuron 1998,
30. van Mier H, Tempel L, Perlmutter JS, Raichle ME, Petersen SE:
21:1226-1227. Generalization of practice-related effects in motor learning using
52. Grant SG, O’Dell TJ, Karl K, Stein PL, Soriano P, Kandel ER:
the dominant and nondominant hand measured by PET. Soc Impaired long-term potentiation, spatial learning, and Neurosci Abstr 1995, 21:1441. hippocampal development in Fyn mutant mice. Science 1992,
31. Ivry RB, Keele SW, Diener HC: Dissociation of the lateral and 258:1903-1910. medial cerebellum in movement timing and movement execution.
53. Abel T, Nguyen PV, Barad M, Deuel TAS, Kandel ER,
Exp Brain Res 1998, 73:167-180.
Bourtchouladze R: Genetic demonstration of a role for PKA in the
32. Penfield W, Milner B: Memory deficits induced by bilateral lesions late phase of LTP and in hippocampus-based long-term memory. in the hippocampal zone. AMA Arch Neurol Psychiatry 1958, Cell 1997, 88:615-626. 79:475-497.
54. Bourtchouladze R, Frenguelli B, Blendy J, Cioffi D, Schutz G, Silva AL:
33. Alvarez P, Squire LR: Memory consolidation and the medial Deficient long-term memory in mice with a targeted mutation of temporal lobe: a simple network model. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA the cAMP-responsive element-binding protein. Cell 1994,
1994, 91:7041-7045. 79:59-68.
34. McClelland JL, McNaughton BL, O’Reilly RC: Why there are
55. Impey S, Mark M, Villacres EC, Poser S, Chavkin C, Storm DR:
complementary learning systems in the hippocampus and Induction of CRE-mediated gene expression by stimuli that neocortex: insights from the successes and failures of generate long-lasting LTP in area CA1 of the hippocampus. connectionist models of learning and memory. Psychol Rev 1995, Neuron 1996, 16:973-982. 102:419-457.
56. Impey S, Smith DM, Obrietan K, Donahue R, Wade C, Storm DR:
35. Squire LR, Alvarez P: Retrograde amnesia and memory Stimulation of cAMP response element (CRE)-mediated consolidation: a neurobiological perspective. Curr Opin Neurobiol transcription during contextual learning. Nat Neurosci 1998,
1995, 5:169-177. 7:595-601.
36. Yin JCP, Tully T: CREB and the formation of long-term memory.
Tsien JZ, Chen DF, Gerber D, Tom C, Mercer EH, Anderson DJ,
Curr Opin Neurobiol 1996, 6:264-268.
Mayford M, Kandel ER, Tonegawa S: Subregion- and cell type- restricted gene knockout in mouse brain. Cell 1996,
Kandel ER, Pittenger C: The past, the future and the biology of 87:1317-1326. memory storage. Philos Trans R Soc Lond B Biol Sci 1999, 354:2027-2052.
58. Tsien JZ, Huerta PT, Tonegawa S: The essential role of hippocampal CA1 NMDA receptor-dependent synaptic plasticity
38. Dash PK, Hochner B, Kandel ER: Injection of the cAMP-responsive in spatial memory. Cell 1996, 87:1327-1338. element into the nucleus of Aplysia sensory neurons blocks long- term facilitation. Nature 1990, 345:718-721.
59. Mayford M, Bach ME, Huang Y-Y, Wang L, Hawkins RD, Kandel ER:
Control of memory formation through regulated expression of a
39. Bartsch D, Casadio A, Karl KA, Serodio P, Kandel ER: CREB1 CaMKII transgene. Science 1996, 274:1678-1683. encodes a nuclear activator, a repressor, and a cytoplasmic modulator that form a regulatory unit critical for long-term
60. Li Y, Erzurumi RS, Chen C, Jhaveri S, Tonegawa S: Whisker-related facilitation. Cell 1998, 95:211-223. neuronal patterns fail to develop in the trigeminal brainstem of NMDAR1 knockout mice. Cell 1994, 76:427-437.
40. Bartsch D, Ghirardi M, Skehel PA, Karl KA, Herder SP, Chen M,
Bailey CH, Kandel ER: Aplysia CREB2 represses long-term
61. Tang YP, Shimizu E, Dube GR, Rampon C, Kerchner GA, Zhuo M,
facilitation: relief of repression converts transient facilitation into
Liu-G, Tsien JZ: Genetic enhancement of learning and memory in long-term functional and structural change. Cell 1995, 83:979-992. mice. Nature 1999, 401:63-69. Cognitive neuroscience Albright, Kandel and Posner 623
62. Gossen M, Freundlieb S, Bender G, Muller G, Hillen W, Bujard H:
86. Reynolds JH, Chelazi L, Desimone R: Competitive mechanisms Transcriptional activation by tetracyclines in mammalian cells. subserve attention in macaque areas V2 and V4. J Neurosci 1999, Science 1995, 268:1766-1769. 19:1736-1753.
63. Mansuy IM, Mayford M, Jacob B, Kandel ER, Bach ME: Restricted
Desimone R, Duncan J: Neural mechanisms of selective visual and regulated overexpression reveals calcineurin as a key attention. Annu Rev Neurosci 1995, 18:193-222. component in the transition from short- to long-term memory. Cell 1998, 92:39-49.
88. Assad WF, Rainer G, Miller EK: Neural activity in the primate prefrontal cortex during associative learning. Neuron 1998,
64. Mansuy IM, Winder DG, Moallem TM, Osman M, Mayford M,
21:1399-1407.
Hawkins RD, Kandel ER: Inducible and reversible gene expression with the rtTA system for the study of memory. Neuron 1998,
89. Glimcher PW, Sparks DL: Movement selection in advance of action 21:257-265. in the superior colliculus. Nature 1992, 355:542-545.
65. Mulkey RM, Endo S, Shenolikar S, Malenka RC: Involvement of a
90. Kim JN, Shadlen MN: Neural correlates of a decision in the calcineurin/inhibitor-1 phosphatase cascade in hippocampal dorsolateral prefrontal cortex of the macaque. Nat Neurosci 1999, long-term depression. Nature 1994, 369:486-488. 2:176-185.
66. Winder DG, Mansuy IM, Osman M, Moallem TM, Kandel ER: Genetic
91. Merzenich MM, Recanzone GH, Jenkins WM, Grajski KA: Adaptive and pharmacological evidence for a novel, intermediate phase of mechanisms in cortical networks underlying cortical contributions long-term potentiation (I−LTP) suppressed by calcineurin. Cell to learning and nondeclarative memory. Cold Spring Harb Symp
1998, 92:25-37. Quant Biol 1990, 55:873-887.
Hebb DO: The Organization of Behavior: A Neuropsychological
92. Gilbert CD, Wiesel TN: Receptive field dynamics in adult primary Theory. New York: Wiley; 1949. visual cortex. Nature 1992, 356:150-152.
68. Albright TD: Motion perception and the mind-body problem. Curr
93. Chino YM, Kaas JH, Smith EL 3rd, Langston AL, Cheng H: Rapid Biol 1991, 1:391-393. reorganization of cortical maps in adult cats following restricted deafferentation in retina. Vision Res 1992, 32:789-796.
69. Newsome WT, Britten KH, Movshon JA: Neuronal correlates of a perceptual decision. Nature 1989, 341:52-54.
94. Darian-Smith C, Gilbert CD: Axonal sprouting accompanies functional reorganization in adult cat striate cortex. Nature 1994,
70. Salzman CD, Britten KH, Newsome WT: Cortical microstimulation 368:737-740. influences perceptual judgements of motion direction. Nature 1990, 346:174-177.
95. Das A, Gilbert CD: Receptive field expansion in adult visual cortex is linked to dynamic changes in strength of cortical connections.
71. Stoner GR, Albright TD, Ramachandran VS: Transparency and J Neurophysiol 1995, 74:779-792. coherence in human motion perception. Nature 1990, 344:153-155.
96. Recanzone CH, Schreiner CE, Merzenich MM: Plasticity in the frequency representation of primary auditory cortex following
72. Stoner GR, Albright TD: The interpretation of visual motion: discrimination training in adult owl monkeys. J Neurosci 1993, evidence for surface segmentation mechanisms. Vision Res 1996, 13:87-103. 36:1291-1310.
Zohary E, Celebrini S, Britten KH, Newsome WT: Neuronal plasticity
73. Duncan RO, Albright TD, Stoner GR: Occlusion and the that underlies improvement in perceptual performance. Science interpretation of visual motion: perceptual and neuronal effects of
1994, 263:1289-1292. context. J Neurosci 2000, 20:5885-5897.
98. Andersen RA, Mountcastle VB: The influence of the angle of gaze
Stoner GR, Albright TD: Neural correlates of perceptual motion upon the excitability of the light-sensitive neurons of the coherence. Nature 1992, 358:412-414. posterior parietal cortex. J Neurosci 1983, 3:532-548.
75. Stoner GR, Albright TD: Image segmentation cues in motion
99. Andersen RA, Essick GK, Siegel RM: Encoding of spatial location processing: implications for modularity in vision. J Cogn Neurosci by posterior parietal neurons. Science 1985, 230:456-458.
1993, 5:129-149.
100. Olson CR, Gettner SN: Object-centered direction selectivity in the
76. Peterhans E, von Der Heydt R: Subjective contours — bridging the macaque supplementary eye field. Science 1995, 269:985-988. gap between psychophysics and physiology. Trends Neurosci 1991, 14:112-119.
101. Graziano MS, Yap GS, Gross CG: Coding of visual space by premotor neurons. Science 1994, 266:1054-1057.
Assad JA, Maunsell JH: Neuronal correlates of inferred motion in primate posterior parietal cortex. Nature 1995, 373:518-521.
102. Gray CM, Konig P, Engel AK, Singer W: Oscillatory responses in cat visual cortex exhibit inter-columnar synchronization which reflects
78. Albright TD: My most true mind thus makes mine eye untrue. global stimulus properties. Nature 1989, 338:334-337. Trends Neurosci 1995, 18:331-333.
103. Cabeza R, Nyberg L: Imaging cognition. II: an empirical analysis of
79. Bushnell MC, Goldberg ME, Robinson DL: Behavioral enhancement 275 PET and fMRI studies. J Cog Neurosci 2000, 12:1-47. of visual responses in monkey cerebral cortex. I. Modulation in posterior parietal cortex related to selective visual attention.
104. Fox PT, Parsons LM, Lancaster JL: Beyond the single study: J Neurophysiol 1981, 46:755-772. function/location metanalysis in cognitive neuroimaging. Curr Opin Neurobiol 1998, 8:178-187.
80. Spitzer H, Desimone R, Moran J: Increased attention enhances both behavioral and neuronal performance. Science 1988, 240:338-340.
105. Lashley KS: Brain Mechanisms and Intelligence: A Quantitative Studyof Injuries to the Brain. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1929.
81. McAdams CJ, Maunsell JHR: Effects of attention on orientation- tuning functions of single neurons in macaque cortical area V4.
106. Van Essen DC, Drury HA, Joshi S, Miller MI: Functional and J Neurosci 1999, 19:431-441. structural mapping of human cerebral cortex: solutions are in the surfaces. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998, 95:788-795.
82. Moran J, Desimone R: Selective attention gates visual processing in the extrastriate cortex. Science 1985, 229:782-784.
107. Tootell RB, Mendola JD, Hadjikhani NK, Ledden PJ, Liu AK, Reppas JB,
Sereno MI, Dale AM: Functional analysis of V3A and related areas
83. Treue S, Maunsell JH: Attentional modulation of visual motion in human visual cortex. J Neurosci 1997, 17:7060-7078. processing in cortical areas MT and MST. Nature 1996, 382:539-541.
108. Posner MI, Gilbert CD: Attention and primary visual cortex. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1999, 96:2585-2587.
84. Connor CE, Preddie DC, Gallant JL, Van Essen DC: Spatial attention effects in macaque area V4. J Neurosci 1997, 17:3201-3214.
109. Ito M, Gilbert CD: Attention modulates contextual influences in the primary visual cortex of alert monkeys. Neuron 1999, 22:593-604.
85. Croner LJ, Albright TD: Segmentation by color influences responses of motion-sensitive neurons in the cortical middle
110. Corbetta M, Miezin FM, Dobmeyer S, Shulman GL, Petersen SE:
temporal visual area. J Neurosci 1999, 19:3935-3951. Selective and divided attention during visual discrimination of A decade of Current neurobiology shape, color, and speed: functional anatomy by positron emission
124. Bush G, Whalen PJ, Rose BR, Jenike MA, McInerney SC, Rauch SL:
tomography. J Neurosci 1991, 11:2388-2402. The counting Stroop: an interference task specialized for functional neuroimaging-validation study with functional MRI.
111. Corbetta M, Kincade MJ, Ollinger JM, McAvoy MP, Shulman GL:
Hum Brain Mapp 1998, 6:270-280. Voluntary orienting is dissociated from target detection in human posterior parietal cortex. Nat Neurosci 2000, 3:292-297.
125. Bush G, Luu P, Posner MI: Cognitive and emotional influences in the anterior cingulate cortex. Trends Cogn Sci 2000, 4:215-222.
112. Gottlieb JP, Kusunoki M, Goldberg ME: The representation of visual salience in monkey parietal cortex. Nature 1998, 391:481-484.
126. Drevets WC, Raichle ME: Reciprocal suppression of regional blood flow during emotional versus higher cognitive processes:
113. Awh E, Jonides J, Smith EE, Schumacher EH, Koeppe RA, Katz S:
implications for interactions between emotion and cognition. Cog Dissociation of storage and rehearsal in verbal working memory: Emotion 1998, 12:353-385. evidence from positron emission tomography. Psychol Sci 1996, 7:25-31.
127. Early TS, Reiman EM, Raichle ME, Spitznagel EL: Left globus pallidus abnormality in never-medicated patients with
114. Hillyard SA, Anllo-Vento L: Event-related brain potentials in the schizophrenia. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1987, 84:561-563. study of visual selective attention. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998, 95:781-787.
128. Dolan RJ, Fletcher P, Frith CD, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS, Grasby PM:
Dopaminergic modulation of impaired cognitive activation in the
115. Abdullaev YG, Bechtereva MP: Neuronal correlate of the higher- anterior cingulate cortex in schizophrenia. Nature 1995, order semantic code in human prefrontal cortex. Int J Psychophysiol 378:180-182.
1993, 14:167-177.
129. Benes F: Alterations of neural circuitry within layer II of anterior
116. Martinez A, Anllo-Vento L, Sereno MI, Frank LR, Buxton RB,
cingulate cortex in schizophrenia. J Psychiatr Res 1999, 33:511-512.
Dubowitz DJ, Wong EC, Hinrichs H, Heinze HJ, Hillyard SA:
130. Gazzaniga MS: The New Cognitive Neurosciences, edn 2. Involvement of striate and extrastriate visual cortical areas in
Cambridge, Massachusetts: MIT Press; 2000. spatial attention. Nat Neurosci 1999, 2:364-369.
131. Jiang Y, Haxby JV, Martin A, Ungerleider LG, Parasuraman R:
117. Worsley KJ, Cao T, Paus M, Petrides M, Evans AC: Application of Complementary neural mechanisms for tracking items in human random field theory to functional connectivity. Hum Brain Mapp working memory. Science 2000, 287:643-646.
1998, 6:364-367.
132. Dehaene S, Naccache L, Le Clec HG, Koechlin E, Muller M,
118. Gevins A: Electrophysiological imaging of brain function. In Brain
Dehaene-Lamertz G, Vad DE, Mortele PF, Le Bihan D: Imaging Mapping: The Methods. Edited by Toga AW, Mazziotta JC. San Diego:
unconscious semantic priming. Nature 1998, 395:597-600.
133. Petersen SE, Van Mier H, Fiez JA, Raichle ME: The effects of
119. Le Bihan D: Diffusion and Perfusion Magnetic Resonance Imaging. practice on the functional anatomy of task performance. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998, 95:853-860.
120. Conturo TE, Lori NF, Cull TS, Akbudak E, Snyder AZ, Shimony JS,
134. Merzenich MM, Jenkins WM: Cortical representation of learned
McKinstry RC, Burton H, Raichle ME: Tracking neuronal fiber behaviors. In Memory Concepts. Edited by Anderson P, Hvalby B, pathways in the living human brain. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2000,
Paulsen B, Hokfelt B. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1995:437-455. 96:10422-10427.
135. Karni A, Meyer G, Rey-Hipolito C, Jezzard P, Adams MM, Turner R,
121. Klingberg T, Hedehus M, Temple E, Salz T, Gabrieli JD, Moseley ME,
Ungerleider LG: The acquisition of skilled motor performance: fast
Poldrack RA: Microstructure of temporo-parietal white matter as a and slow experience-driven changes in primary motor cortex. basis for reading ability: evidence from diffusion tensor magnetic Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 1998, 95:861-868. resonance imaging. Neuron 2000, 25:493-500.
136. Kim KH, Relkin NR, Lee KM, Hisch J: Distinct cortical areas
122. Dronkers NF: A new brain region for coordinating speech associated with native and second languages. Nature 1997, articulation. Nature 1996, 384:159–161. 388:171-174.
123. Raichle ME, Fiez JA, Videen TO, MacLeod A-MK, Pardo JV, Fox PT,
137. Pantev ET, Wienbruch C, Rockstroh B, Taub E: Increased cortical
Petersen SE: Practice-related changes in human brain functional representation of the fingers of the left hand in string players. anatomy during non-motor learning. Cereb Cortex 1994, 4:8-26. Science 1995, 270:305-307.
How To Steal Code Inventing The Wheel Only Once Much is said about ‘‘standing on other people’s shoulders, not their toes’’, but infact the wheel is re-invented every day in the Unix/C community. Worse, often it is re-invented badly, with bumps, corners, and cracks. There are ways of avoiding this: someof them bad, some of them good, most of them under-appreciated and under-used.
Inhaltsverzeichnis Konsolidierung in vollem Gange – Seite 10-14Ausgewählte Merger- und Übernahmeszenarien Executive Summary Die Entwicklung zur „customized medicine“ schreitet mitder sprunghaften Verbesserung diagnostischer Testver-Der Life Sciences-Sektor wird weiterhin zu den innovations-fahren spürbar voran. Die Basis hierfür liefern die Ergebnissegetriebenen und wachstums